PIN welcomes primatologists who are working directly with species to send updates for our fact sheets any time, including sources. We also welcome all readers to send updates and sources for consideration: we will check with the experts before adding these updates. We advise readers to use our fact sheets as just one source of information and to always research additional sources.
TAXONOMY
Suborder: Haplorrhini
Infraorder: Simiiformes
Superfamily: Cercopithecoidea
Family: Cercopithecidae
Subfamily: Cercopithecinae
Genus: Macaca
Species: M. fuscata
Subspecies: M. f. fuscata, M. f. yakui
Other names: Japanese macaque, Japanese monkey, Snow monkey; macaque japonais, macaque à face rouge (French); japansk makak (Swedish); macaca japonesa (Spanish); Nihon zaru (Japanese) (for vernacular names see Fooden & Aimi 2005:77); M. fuscata yakui: Yaku monkey; Yakushimazaru, Yakuzaru (Japanese).
Total population: approx. 100,000
Regions: Japan
Gestation: 171 days
Height: 570.1 mm (M), 522.8 mm (F)
Weight: 11.3 kg (M), 8.4 kg (F)
Two subspecies of Japanese macaque have been described, Macaca fuscata fuscata and Macaca fuscata yakui. M. f. fuscata is the mainland subspecies of the Japanese macaque. M. f. yakui is restricted to the island of Yakushima at the southern limit of Macaca fuscata distribution and is distinguishable from M. fuscata due to differences in pelage and body size (Fooden & Aimi 2005).
MORPHOLOGY
The fur of the Japanese macaque can vary among shades of brown from grey or yellow brown to exclusively brown (Fooden & Aimi 2005). Japanese macaques have a pinkish face and posterior with a short stump of a tail (Rowe 1996). There is a degree of sexual dimorphism between male and female Japanese macaques with the males weighing somewhat more than the females and on average having a longer body length than the females. The average body weight of the Japanese monkey is 11.3 kg (24.91 lb) (M) and 8.4 kg (18.52 lb) (F) (Fooden & Aimi 2005). In addition, there appears to be a correlation between climate and body weight, as Japanese monkeys from northern colder areas weigh more than those from warmer areas of Japan (Hamada et al. 1996). Average body length for males is 570.1 mm (22.44 in) and for females is 522.8 mm (20.58 in) (Fooden & Aimi 2005). The tail is short, averaging 92.51 mm (3.64 in) in males and 79.08 mm (3.11 in) in females (Hamada et al. 1996). Longevity for males has been confirmed as long as 28 years and into the 32nd year of life for females (Fedigan 1991; Nakamichi et al. 1995; Fedigan & Pavelka 2001). Actual average lifespan, however, is significantly shorter; with wild females living for an average of just 6.3 years (Takahata et al. 1998).
Japanese macaques are predominantly quadrupedal and can be considered semi-terrestrial. Females spend more time in trees than males and males spend more time on the ground than females. Leaping is seen in the Japanese macaque, but suspension from supports is not common (Chatani 2003). Japanese macaques are excellent swimmers and are reportedly able to swim distances of over half a kilometer (Mito 1980 cited in Fooden & Aimi 2005). This macaque’s fur is a unique adaptation to cold, as fur thickness increases as habitat temperature decreases and allows the monkey to cope with winter temperatures as low as -20° C (-4° F) (Hori et al. 1977).
RANGE
CURRENT RANGE MAPS (IUCN REDLIST):
Macaca fuscata
The Japanese macaque is native to the islands of Japan. Of the four main Japanese islands, Hokkaido, Honshu, Shikoku, and Kyushu, Japanese macaques are found on all but Hokkaido, the northernmost (Fooden & Aimi 2005). Even though its range is restricted to the three southern main islands of Japan, the species nevertheless represents the northernmost wild populations of non-human primates in the world. The absolute northernmost Japanese macaque populations are found at the northern tip of the island of Honshu on the Shimokita peninsula (Izawa & Nishida 1963; Uehara 1975). These northern populations exhibit significant cold adaptation as northern Honshu can be snowbound for up to a third of the year (Izawa & Nishida 1963). Japanese macaques also inhabit several smaller islands near the coasts of Honshu, Shikoku, and Kyushu (Fooden & Aimi 2005).
The southernmost population of Japanese macaques is that on Yakushima Island off of the south coast of Japan and has been assigned its own subspecies, M.f.yakui (Uehara 1975; Hanya 2004). In February 1972, a complete group of Japanese macaques numbering 150 individuals was transplanted from Arashiyama, near Kyoto on Honshu to Laredo, Texas where it was established in an arid brushland habitat and grew, having reached 470 individuals by 1989 (Fedigan 1991). The total population of Japanese macaques has been estimated to be 114,431 individuals and likely numbers around 100,000 individuals in its natural habitat (Hashiba 1989; Fooden & Aimi 2005). The Japanese macaque is rarely found in lowland areas owing to the high levels of human habitation in those areas (Amagasa & Ito 1978 cited in Fooden & Aimi 2005). The Japanese macaque has been studied in the wild longer than most primate species, having been observed in numerous habitats and locations throughout Japan for over 50 years (Yamagiwa & Hill 1998). Since the 1950’s, study of Japanese macaques has been undertaken at Arashiyama, near Kyoto (Huffman 1991a). This long-term study site has not only served to provide information as to the long-term changes within a Japanese macaque population, but also to serve as the source of the introduced Arashiyama West troop in Texas which has provided not only a study site for the Japanese macaque in north America, but a place to study adaptations of the species to a new habitat (Fedigan 1991). Within their range, the distribution of the Japanese macaque is expanding, at least partially as a result of the species losing their fear of humans, coupled with demographic and economic shifts in the Japanese human population (Watanabe & Muroyama 2005). This expansion may be misleading as to the status of the species however, because within the range, the total population is significantly limited by the distribution of the natural broadleaf forest that is available. In addition, the expansion of the range distribution was found to be correlated with the reduction of forests (Hashiba 1989).
HABITAT
Due to variations in the latitude and altitude of the Japanese islands, the habitat of the Japanese macaque varies greatly between the extremes of its distribution. Near the southern end of the population distribution, habitats include sub-tropical forest and at the northernmost reaches, sub-arctic forest is found in mountainous regions. Between these two extremes, both warm and cool temperate forests are found (Uehara 1975). These types of forest include both the deciduous forests of central and northern Japan and the broadleaf evergreen forests of the southwest of the islands. As is expected in a variable range, there is a large spectrum of vegetation within the distribution of the Japanese macaque (Suzuki 1965). The highest elevation at which the Japanese macaque has been reported is 3180 m (10,433 ft) (Izumiyama pers. comm. cited in Fooden & Aimi 2005). Of the habitats of the Japanese macaque, the two most important are the warm temperate evergreen broadleaf forest and the cool temperate deciduous broadleaf forest (Fooden & Aimi 2005). In the northern deciduous forests, leaves are absent from the trees for up to 5 months of the year (Suzuki 1965). Near the northern end of the range in the cool temperate deciduous broadleaf forest on the island of Kinkazan, annual rainfall averages 1500mm (59.06 in) with an average temperature of 11° C (51.8° F). At the extreme southern end of the macaque range on the island of Yakushima in warm temperate evergreen broadleaf forest, annual rainfall averages 3000mm (118.11 in) with an average temperature of 20° C (68° F). During the winter in high altitude or latitude areas of the Japanese macaque range, snow is often quite deep, having been recorded on the Shimokita peninsula and in the Shiga Heights in central Honshu to be between 2-3 meters deep during the winter (Izawa & Nishida 1963; Hori et al. 1977).
ECOLOGY
The omnivorous Japanese macaque is not a picky eater and will eat a great variety of foods including over 213 species of plant (Koganezawa 1975). Other main foods eaten include insects and soil (Koganezawa 1975). As expected, variable types of foods are available in different habitats and in some cases food availability is profoundly seasonal. In the south of the range in temperate evergreen broadleaf forest of Yakushima island, fruit predominates in the diet. At this location, the macaques will eat fungi, ferns, invertebrates, soil and other parts of plants, but the main foods were fruits, mature leaves, and fallen seeds (Maruhashi 1980; Agetsuma & Nakagawa 1998). Seasonality of the diet is seen on Yakushima, with a greater number of fruits being eaten in the summer and more herbs being eaten in the winter (Hanya 2004). In the deciduous broadleaf forests habitat of the north where food is scarce in the winter, the species must eat surplus high quality foods such as fruits and nuts prior to winter to store enough fat to survive through the lean winter (Hanya et al. 2006). This storage of fat is important because during snowbound winter months the only available foods are dead and poor in nutrition, including leaves and bark (Tsuji et al. 2006). In the deciduous broadleaf forest habitat on the northern island of Kinkazan, food items are mainly fallen seeds, herbs, young leaves and fruits (Agetsuma & Nakagawa 1998). The Japanese macaque has been observed digging up underground plant parts such as roots, eating soil, and consuming raw fish if other preferred food items are not to be found (Koganezawa 1975; Watanabe 1989; Iguchi & Izawa 1990).
The average group size of the Japanese macaque is 41 individuals but they can range from 10 to as large as 161 (Fooden & Aimi 2005). If provisioned, groups are typically larger than wild groups and in an extreme example a provisioned group grew to over a thousand individuals (Sugiyama & Ohsawa 1988; Fooden & Aimi 2005). The home range averages around 3.7 km² (1.43 mi²) and correlates with habitat type (Fooden & Aimi 2005). Home range decreases with access to cultivated land, as this provides concentrated food sources which the macaque troops can utilize (Izumiyama et al. 2003). There is also pronounced seasonality of daily path length, even in the southern reaches of the Japanese macaque range on Yakushima. At this location, the daily path decreased from an average of 2.0 km (1.24 mi) per day during August and September to 1.2 km (.75 mi) per day later in winter (Maruhashi 1979 cited in Maruhashi 1981). In habitats further north during the winter, movement is even further restricted, averaging only .577 km (.36mi) for females and .529km (.33 mi) for males (Koganesawa 1986). As less food is available in the winter and more in the summer and fall, daily travel seems to be directly influenced by food availability; with less food there less travel (Yotsumoto 1976).
The Japanese macaque is diurnal. Daily activities vary with the seasons as well as with habitat. In colder areas during the summer and fall when food is abundant, discrete daily patterns of activity emerge with little variation. In the winter and spring when less food is available, levels of activity fall and vary significantly from day to day (Yotsumoto 1976). In the autumn to early winter, the daily activities are characterized by three bouts of feeding separated by other activities. In the winter, two to four periods of feeding are observed with conspicuously less daily activity. Finally, in the spring and summer, there are two or three bouts of feeding per day (Yotsumoto 1976). The macaques typically sleep in trees but they also sleep on the ground, often on flat rocks and fallen trees or behind rocks or fallen trees (Wada & Tokida 1985 cited in Fooden & Aimi 2005; Takahashi 1997). In the winter, Japanese macaques will huddle together on the ground for warmth in sleeping groups which grow larger with lower temperatures (Takahashi 1997). In warmer areas of the Japanese macaque range on the island of Yakushima, daily activities were highly variable but time is spent on average, 20.9% inactive, 22.8% traveling, 23.5% feeding, 27.9% social grooming, 1.2% self-grooming, and 3.7% other activities (Maruhashi 1981). In Shiga Heights in central Japan, the macaques enter and remain in hot springs in the winter, probably to regulate their body temperature behaviorally (Hori et al. 1977).
Japanese macaques are preyed upon by several species including feral dogs and Mountain Hawk Eagles (Spizaetus nipalensis) (Miyamoto 1976 cited in Fooden & Aimi 2005; Iida 1999). In addition, raccoon dogs (Nyctereutes procyonoides) may be a predator of the Japanese macaques, especially infants (Iwamoto 1974). Japanese macaques are also killed in Japan when their crop-raiding and interactions with humans become a problem. The annual kill surpassed 10,000 individuals annually in 1998 (Watanabe & Muroyama 2005). This is significant because this annual kill represents around a tenth of the entire estimated population.
Content last modified: April 26, 2007
Written by Kurt Gron. Reviewed by Sarah Turner.
Cite this page as:
Gron KJ. 2007 April 26. Primate Factsheets: Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata) Taxonomy, Morphology, & Ecology . <http://pin.primate.wisc.edu/factsheets/entry/japanese_macaque/taxon>. Accessed 2020 July 21.
INTERNATIONAL STATUS
For individual primate species conservation status, please search the IUCN Red List.
Also search the current scientific literature for primate conservation status (overall as well as for individual species), and visit CITES (Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species of Wild Fauna and Flora).
Conservation information last updated in 2007 follows, for comparison:
CONSERVATION THREATS
Threat: Human Induced Habitat Loss and Degradation
The Japanese macaque has arrived at somewhat of a crossroads in relation to its conservation and the threats to its survival. Since the Second World War, traditional threats to the species such as slash-and-burn agriculture, use of forest woods for construction and fuel, and hunting decreased significantly, mainly due to social and economic changes in Japan (Sprague 2002). In this regard, the outlook for conservation of the Japanese macaque looks significantly brighter. On the other hand, with such a shift from traditional threats, new conflicts with the species have emerged. The two most serious of these new threats are the replacement of natural forests with lumber plantations and the rise in crop-raiding by the Japanese macaque (Sprague 2002). The lumber plantations cannot be used as habitat by the monkeys and the rise in crop-raiding has resulted in the species being considered a nuisance. Behind wild boar and deer, the Japanese macaque is the third worst crop-raider in Japan (Sprague 2002). As a result, they have less appropriate habitat in which to live coupled with yearly culling of the species as a pest in excess of 10,000 individuals per annum (Watanabe & Muroyama 2005).
Apparently, the range of the species is expanding but whether or not the total number of Japanese macaques is expanding or contracting is unclear. The range expansion is attributed to behavioral changes within the Japanese macaque population in which they have lost their fear of humans and do not actively avoid people (Watanabe & Muroyama 2005). This lack of fear has resulted in a Japanese macaque presence in not only rural and agricultural areas, but an increasing presence in urban areas. In one extreme case, a female was living in central Tokyo for several months (Fukuda 2004).
In 1993, part of Yakushima island, the habitat of M. f. yakui, was designated a Natural World Heritage Site by UNESCO which has proven to have had mixed results for conservation at that locale (Hill & Maruhashi 1996/1997). The designation, while raising awareness of a need for conservation, has brought additional tourists to the threatened habitat and has not as of yet proven adequate for protection of the Japanese macaques of Yakushima island (Hill & Maruhashi 1996/1997).
LINKS TO MORE ABOUT CONSERVATION
CONSERVATION INFORMATION
- No current links for Macaca fuscata
- Links for all species
CONSERVATION NEWS
- Look, but don’t stare! Hanging out with Kyoto’s macaques (CNN; May 9, 2013)
- Links for all species
ORGANIZATIONS INVOLVED IN Macaca fuscata CONSERVATION
Content last modified: April 26, 2007
Written by Kurt Gron. Reviewed by Sarah Turner.
Cite this page as:
Gron KJ. 2007 April 26. Primate Factsheets: Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata) Conservation . <http://pin.primate.wisc.edu/factsheets/entry/japanese_macaque/cons>. Accessed 2020 July 22.
The following references were used in the writing of this factsheet. To find current references for Macaca fuscata, search PrimateLit.
REFERENCES
Aetsuma N, Nakagawa N. 1998. Effects of habitat differences on feeding behaviors of Japanese monkeys: comparison between Yakushima and Kinkazan. Primates 39(3):275-89.
Bardi M, Shimizu K, Fujita S, Borgognini-Tarli S, Huffman MA. 2001. Social behavior and hormonal correlates during the perinatal period in Japanese macaques. Horm Behav 39(3):239-46.
Blount B. 1985. “Girney” vocalizations among Japanese macaque females: context and function. Primates 26(4):424-35.
Chatani K. 2003. Positional behavior of free-ranging Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Primates 44(1):13-23.
Eaton G. 1972. Snowball construction by a feral troop of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) living under seminatural conditions. Primates 13(4):411-4.
Fedigan LM, Griffin L. Determinants of reproductive seasonality in the Arashiyama west Japanese macaques. In: Fa JE, Lindburg DG, editors. Evolution and ecology of macaque societies. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge U Pr. p 369-88.
Fedigan LM, Pavelka MSM. 2001. Is there adaptive value to reproductive termination in Japanese macaques? A test of maternal investment hypotheses. Int J Primatol 22(2):109-25.
Fedigan LM. 1991. History of the Arashiyama west Japanese macaques in Texas. In: Fedigan LM & Asquith PJ, editors. The monkeys of Arashiyama: thirty-five years of research in Japan and the west. Albany (NY): SUNY Pr. p54-69.
Fedigan L. 1991. Life span and reproduction in Japanese macaque females. In: Fedigan LM & Asquith PJ, editors. The monkeys of Arashiyama: thirty-five years of research in Japan and the west. Albany (NY): SUNY Pr. p140-54.
Fedigan LM, Zohar S. 1997. Sex differences in mortality of Japanese macaques: twenty-one years of data from the Arashiyama west population. Am J Phys Anth 102(2):161-75.
Fooden J, Aimi M. 2003. Birth-season variation in Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata. Primates 44(2):109-17.
Fooden J, Aimi M. 2005. Systematic review of Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata (Gray, 1870). Fieldiana: Zoology 104:1-200.
Fujita S, Sugiura H, Mitsunaga F, Shimizu K. 2004. Hormone profiles and reproductive characteristics in wild female Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Am J Primatol 64(4):367-75.
Fukuda F. 2004. Dispersal and environmental disturbance in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Prim Rep 68:53-69.
Fukuda F. 1982. Relationship between age and troop shifting in male Japanese monkeys. Jap J Ecol 32(4): 491-8.
Furuya Y. 1969. On the fission of troops of Japanese monkeys: II. General view of troop fission of Japanese monkeys. Primates 10:47-69.
Glick BB, Eaton GG, Johnson DF, Worlein J. Social behavior of infant and mother Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): effects of kinship, partner sex, and infant sex. Intl J Primatol 7(2):139-55.
Gouzoules H, Goy RW. 1983. Physiological and social influences on mounting behavior of troop-living female monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Am J Primatol 5(1):39-49.
Gouzoules H. 1984. Social relations of males and infants in a troop of Japanese monkeys: a consideration of causal mechanisms. In: Taub DM, editor. Primate paternalism. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Co. p127-45.
Groves C. 2001. Primate taxonomy. Washington DC: Smithsonian Inst Pr. 350 p.
Hamada Y, Watanabe T, Iwamoto M. 1996. Morphological variations among local populations of Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata). In: Shotake T, Wada K, editors. Variations in the asian macaques. Tokyo: Tokai Univ Pr. p97-115.
Hanby JP, Brown CE. 1974. The development of sociosexual behaviours in Japanese macaques Macaca fuscata. Behaviour 49:152-96.
Hanby JP, Robertson LT, Phoenix CH. 1971. The sexual behavior of a confined troop of Japanese macaques. Folia Primatol 16:123-43.
Hanya G. 2004. Diet of a Japanese macaque troop in the coniferous forest of Yakushima. Intl J Primatol 25(1):55-69.
Hanya G, Kiyono M, Yamada A, Suzuki K, Furukawa M, Yoshida Y, Chijiiwa A. 2006. Not only annual food abundance but also fallback food quality determines the Japanese macaque density: evidence from seasonal variations in home range size. Primates 47(3):275-8.
Hashiba K. 1989. Population estimation of Japanese macaques for conservation. Reichorui Kenkyu (Primate Research) 5(1):22-35.
Hill DA, Maruhashi T. 1996/1997. Conservation of Japanese macaques in Yakushima: the effectiveness of UNESCO’s natural world heritage designation. Prim Conserv (17):160-3
Hiraiwa M. 1981. Maternal and alloparental care in a troop of free-ranging Japanese monkeys. Primates 22(3):309-29.
Hori T, Nakayama T, Tokura H, Hara F, Suzuki M. 1977. Thermoregulation of the Japanese macaque living in a snowy mountain area. Jap J Physiol 27:305-19.
Huffman MA. 1991a. History of the Arashiyama Japanese macaques in Kyoto, Japan. In: Fedigan LM, Asquith PJ, editors. The monkeys of Arashiyama: thirty-five years of research in Japan and the west. Albany (NY):SUNY Pr. p21-53.
Huffman MA. 1992. Influences of female partner preference on potential reproductive outcome in Japanese macaques. Folia Primatol 59(2):77-88.
Huffman MA. 1991b. Mate selection and partner preferences in female Japanese macaques. In: Fedigan LM, Asquith PJ, editors. The monkeys of Arashiyama: thirty-five years of research in Japan and the west. Albany (NY): SUNY Pr. p101-22.
Huffman MA, Quiatt D. 1986. Stone handling by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): implications for tool use of stone. Primates 27(4):413-23.
Iida T. 1999. Predation of Japanese macaque Macaca fuscata by mountain hawk eagle Spizaetus nipalensis. Jap J Ornithol 47(3):125-7.
Iguchi M, Izawa K. 1990. Digging and eating of underground plant-parts by wild Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Primates 31(4):621-4.
Itani J. 1959. Paternal care in the wild Japanese monkey, Macaca fuscata fuscata. Primates 2:61-93.
Itoigawa N, Tanaka T, Ukai N, Fujii H, Kurokawa T, Koyama T, Ando A, Watanabe Y, Imakawa S. 1992. Demography and reproductive parameters of a free-ranging group of Japanese Macaques (Macaca fuscata) at Katsuyama. Primates 33(1):49-68.
Iwamoto T. 1974. A bioeconomic study on a provisioned troop of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata fuscata) at Koshima islet, Miyazaki. Primates 15(2-3):241-62.
Izawa K, Nishida T. 1963. Monkeys living in the northern limits of their distribution. Primates 4(2):67-88.
Izumiyama S, Mochizuki T, Shiraishi T. 2003. Troop size, home range area and seasonal range use of the Japanese macaque in the northern Japan alps. Ecol Res 18(5):465-74.
Kawai M, Azuma S, Yoshiba K. 1967. Ecological studies of reproduction in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata): I. Problems of the birth season. Primates 8:35-74.
Kawai M. 1965. Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima islet. Primates 6(1):1-30.
Kawanaka K. 1973. Intertroop relationships among Japanese monkeys. Primates 14(2-3):113-59.
Koganezawa M. 1974. Food habits of Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata) in the Boso mountains. In: Kondo S, Kawai M, Ehara A, editors. Contemporary primatology, proceedings of the 5th International Congress of Primatology. Basel(CH):S. Karger. p380-3.
Koganezawa M. 1986. Winter movement and spatial distribution of Japanese monkeys in Nikko, Japan. Prim Rep (15):31-9.
Koyama N. 1991. Grooming relationships in the Arashiyama group of Japanese monkeys. In: Fedigan LM, Asquith PJ, editors. The monkeys of Arashiyama: thirty-five years of research in Japan and the west. Albany (NY): SUNY Pr. p211-26.
Koyama NF. 2003. Matrilineal cohesion and social networks in Macaca fuscata. Intl J Primatol 24(4):797-811.
Koyama N. 1967. On dominance rank and kinship of a wild Japanese monkey troop in Arashiyama. Primates 8:189-216.
Koyama N, Takahata Y, Huffman MA, Norikoshi K, Suzuki H. 1992. Reproductive parameters of female Japanese macaques: thirty years data from the Arashiyama troops, Japan. Primates 33(1):33-47.
Machida S. 1990. Threat calls in alliance formation by members of a captive group of Japanese monkeys. Primates 31(2):205-11.
Majolo B, Schino G, Troisi A. 2005. Towards thirty years of ethological research on the Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata) colony of the Rome zoo: a review. J Anth Sci 83:43-60.
Maruhashi T. 1981. Activity patterns of a troop of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata yakui) on Yakushima island, Japan. Primates 22(1):1-14.
Maruhashi T. 1980. Feeding behavior and diet of the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata yakui) on Yakushima island, Japan. Primates 21(2):141-60.
Maruhashi T. 1992. Fission, takeover, and extinction of a troop of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata yakui) on Yakushima island, Japan. In: Itoigawa N, Sugiyama Y, Sackett GP, Thompson RKR, editors. Topics in primatology, volume 2: behavior, ecology, and conservation. Tokyo: U Tokyo Pr. p47-65.
Maruhashi T. 1979. Natural life of the yakuzaru (Macaca fuscata yakui), group-movement and sub-grouping. Monkey 23(4):6-15.
Masataka N, Fujii H. 1980. An experimental study on facial expressions and interindividual distance in Japanese macaques. Primates 21(3):340-9.
Masataka N. 1989. Motivational referents of contact calls in Japanese monkeys. Ethology 80(1-4):265-73.
Matsubara M, Sprague DS. 2004. Mating tactics in response to costs incurred by mating with multible males in wild female Japanese macaques. Intl J Primatol 25(4):901-17.
Minami T. 1974. Early mother-infant relations in Japanese monkeys. In: Kondo S, Kawai M, Ehara A, editors. Contemporary primatology, proceedings of the 5th International Congress of Primatology. Basel(CH):S. Karger. p334-340.
Mitani M. 1986. Voiceprint identification and its application to sociological studies of wild Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata yakui). Primates 27(4):397-412.
Mito S. 1980. Bosuzaru heno Michi. Tokyo: Popura-sha. 166 p.
Miyamoto M. 1976. New wolves? Feral dogs in Omote Nikko. Anima 45:6-11.
Muroyama Y. 1995. Developmental changes in mother-offspring grooming in Japanese macaques. Am J Primatol 37(1):57-64.
Nakamichi M, Shizawa Y. 2003. Distribution of grooming among adult females in a large, free-ranging group of Japanese macaques. Intl J Primatol 24(3):607-25.
Nakamichi M, Kojima Y, Itoigawa N, Imakawa S, Machida S. 1995. Interactions among adult males and females before and after the death of the alpha male in a free-ranging troop of Japanese macaques. Primates 36(3):385-96.
Nakamichi M, Imakawa S, Kojima Y, Natsume A. 1992. Parturition in a free-ranging Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata). Primates 33(3):413-8.
Nakamichi M. 1989. Sex differences in social development during the first 4 years in a free-ranging group of Japanese monkeys, Macaca fuscata. Anim Behav 38(5):737-48.
Negayama K, Kondo K, Itoigawa N. 1983. Development of locomotor behaviour in infant Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Annal Sci Nat, Zool, Paris. (Ser. 13)5(3):169-80.
Nigi H. 1982. The Japanese monkey-its physiology of sex. Tokyo:Dobutsu-sha. 309 p.
Nigi H. 1975. Menstrual cycle and some other related aspects of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Primates 16(2):207-16.
O’Neill AC, Fedigan LM, Ziegler TE. 2004. Ovarian cycle phase and same-sex mating behavior in Japanese macaque females. Am J Primatol 63(1):25-31.
Ôta K, Makino Y, Kimura M, Suzuki J. 1991. Lactation in the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata): yield and composition of milk and nipple preference of young. Primates 32(1):35-48.
Pereira ME. 1986. Maternal recognition of juvenile offspring coo vocalizations in Japanese macaques. Animal Behaviour 34(3):935-7.
Perloe SI. 1992. Male mating competition, female choice and dominance in a free ranging group of Japanese macaques. Primates 33(3):289-304.
Rowe N. 1996. The pictorial guide to the living primates. East Hampton (NY): Pogonias Pr. p124-5.
Saito C, Sato S, Suzuki S, Sugiura H, Agetsuma N, Takahata Y, Sasaki C, Takahashi H, Tanaka T, Yamagiwa J. 1998. Aggressive intergroup encounters in two populations of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Primates 39(3):303-12.
Sakura O. 1989. Variability in contact calls between troops of Japanese macaques: a possible case of neutral evolution of animal culture. Anim Behav 38(5):900-2.
Shizawa Y, Nakamichi M, Hinobayashi T, Minami T. 2005. Playback experiment to test maternal responses of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) to their own infantÕs call when the infants were four to six months old. Behav Process 68:41-6.
Soltis J. 1999. Measuring male-female relationships during the mating season in wild Japanese macaques. Primates 40(3):453-67.
Sprague DS, Suzuki S, Takahashi H, Sato S. 1998. Male life history in natural populations of Japanese macaques: migration, dominance rank, and troop participation of males in two habitats. Primates 39(3):351-63.
Sprague DS. 1991. Mating by nontroop males among the Japanese macaques of Yakushima island. Folia Primatol 57(3):156-8.
Sprague DS. 2002. Monkeys in the backyard: encroaching wildlife and rural communities in Japan. In: Fuentes A, Wolfe LD, editors. Primates face to face: conservation implications of human-nonhuman primate interconnections. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge U Pr. p254-72.
Sprague DS, Suzuki S, Tsukahara T. 1996. Variation in social mechanisms by which males attained the alpha rank among Japanese macaques. In: Fa JE, Lindburg DG, editors. Evolution and ecology of macaque societies. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge U Pr. p 444-58.
Sugiura H. 2001. Vocal exchange of coo calls in Japanese macaques. In:Matsuzawa T, editor. Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Tokyo: Springer. p135-54.
Sugiyama Y. 1976. Life history of male Japanese monkeys. Adv Stud Behav 7:255-84.
Sugiyama Y, Ohsawa H. 1988. Population dynamics and management of baited Japanese monkeys at Takasakiyama. Reichorui Kenkyu (Primate Research) 4(1):33-43.
Suzuki A. 1965. An ecological study of wild Japanese monkeys in snowy areas-focused on their food habits. Primates 6(1):31-72.
Takahashi H. 2002. Changes of dominance rank, age, and tenure of wild Japanese macaque males in the Kinkazan A troop during seven years. Primates 43(2):133-8.
Takahashi H. 1997. Huddling relationships in night sleeping groups among wild Japanese macaques in Kinkazan island during winter. Primates 38(1):57-68.
Takahata Y. Diachronic changes in the dominance relations of adult female Japanese monkeys of the Arashiyama B group. In: Fedigan LM & Asquith PJ, editors. The monkeys of Arashiyama: Thirty-five years of research in Japan and the west. Albany(NY): SUNY Pr. p123-39.
Takahata Y, Suzuki S, Agetsuma N, Okayasu N, Sugiura H, Takahashi H, Yamagiwa J, Izawa K, Furuichi T, Hill DA, Maruhashi T, Saito C, Sato S, Sprague DS. 1998. Reproduction of wild Japanese macaque females of Yakushima and Kinkazan islands: a preliminary report. Primates 39(3):339-349.
Takahata Y, Huffman MA, Suzuki S, Koyama N, Yamagiwa J. 1999. Why dominants do not consistently attain high mating and reproductive success: a review of longitudinal Japanese macaque studies. Primates 40(1):143-58.
Tanaka I. 1995. Matrilineal distribution of louse egg-handling techniques during grooming in free-ranging Japanese macaques. Am J Phys Anth 98(2):197-201.
Tanaka I, Hayama S-I, Nigi H. 1993. Milk secretion in pregnancy among free-ranging Japanese macaques. Am J Primatol 30(2):169-74.
Thomsen R. 1997. Observation of periparturitional behaviour in wild Yakushima macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui). Folia Primatol 68(6):338-41.
Tsuji Y, Fujita S, Sugiura H, Saito C, Takatsuki S. 2006. Long-term variation in fruiting and the food habits of wild Japanese macaques on Kinkazan island, northern Japan. Am J Primatol 68(11):1068-80.
Tsukahara T. 1990. Initiation and solicitation in male-female grooming in a wild Japanese macaque troop on Yakushima island. Primates 31(2):147-56.
Uehara S. 1975. The importance of the temperate forest elements among woody food plants utilized by Japanese monkeys and its possible historical meaning for the establishment of the monkeys’ range: a preliminary report. In: Kondo S, Kawai M, Ehara A, editors. Contemporary primatology, proceedings of the 5th International Congress of Primatology. Basel(CH):S. Karger. p392-400.
Vasey PL, Foroud A, Duckworth N, Kovacovsky SD. 2006. Male-female and female-female mounting in Japanese macaques: a comparative study of posture and movement. Arch Sex Behav 35(2):117-29.
Ventura R, Majolo B, Schino G, Hardie S. 2005. Differential effects of ambient temperature and humidity on allogrooming, self-grooming, and scratching in wild Japanese macaques. Am J Phys Anth 126(4):453-7.
Wada K, Tokida E. 1985. Sleeping site cluster of Japanese monkeys in Shiga A1 troop. In: Wada K, Kawamura S, Matsuzawa T, editors. The studies of spatial distribution of the Japanese monkey troops in Shiga Heights. Kyoto: Prim Res Inst, Kyoto U, Inuyama. p2-10.
Watanabe K. 1989. Fish: a new addition to the diet of Japanese macaques on Koshima island. Folia Primatol 52(3-4):124-31.
Watanabe K, Muroyama Y. 2005. Recent expansion of the range of Japanese macaques, and associated management problems. In: Paterson JD, Wallis J, editors. Commensalism and conflict: the human-primate interface. Norman (OK): Am Soc Primatol. p400-19.
Wolfe L. 1981. Display behavior of three troops of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Primates 22(1):24-32.
Wolfe L. 1979. Sexual maturation among members of a transported troop of Japanese macaques. Primates 20(3):411-8.
Yamada K, Nakamichi M. 2006. A fatal attack on an unweaned infant by a non-resident male in a free-ranging group of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) at Katsuyama. Primates 47(2):165-9.
Yamagiwa J, Hill DA. 1998. Intraspecific variation in the social organization of Japanese macaques: past and present scope of field studies in natural habitats. Primates 39(3):257-73.
Yamagiwa J, Izawa K, Maruhashi T. 1998. Long-term studies on wild Japanese macaques in natural habitats at Kinkazan and Yakushima: preface. Primates 39(3):255-6.
Yoshida A. 1988a. Response to experimental playbacks of conspecific alarm call in free-ranging Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Bull Fac Hum Sci Osaka Univ:91-100.
Yoshida A. 1988b. Alarm and estrous calls of a female Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata): A case study. In: Anonymous, editor. Research reports of the Arashiyama west and east groups of Japanese monkeys. Suita, Japan: Lab Ethol Stud, Fac Hum Sci, Osaka Univ. p57-65.
Yoshimi I, Takasaki H. 2003. Long distance mobility of male Japanese macaques evidenced by mitochondrial DNA. Primates 44(1):71-4.
Yotsumoto N. 1976. The daily activity rhythm in a troop of wild Japanese monkey. Primates 17(2):183-204.
Content last modified: April 26, 2007
VIDEO & WEBCAMS
- Snow Monkeys (3:02, narrated; National Geographic Kids; Flash)
- Wet ‘n’ Wild (1:12, narrated; National Geographic Kids; Flash)
IMAGES
Macaca fuscata Photo: Alisha Eisert |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Alisha Eisert |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Alisha Eisert |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Alisha Eisert |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Frans de Waal |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Frans de Waal |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Frans de Waal |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Frans de Waal |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Frans de Waal |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Frans de Waal |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Frans de Waal |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Frans de Waal |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Frans de Waal |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Frans de Waal |
|
Macaca fuscata Photo: Marilyn Cole |
Primate Info Net (PIN) is maintained by the Wisconsin National Primate Research Center (WNPRC) at the University of Wisconsin-Madison, with countless grants and contributions from others over time. PIN is an ever-growing community effort: if you’d like to contribute, or have questions, please don’t hesitate to contact us.